This is already a historic election year, in which a woman and
an African-American man emerged as the leading candidates to become the
Democratic presidential nominee. PAW’s Mark F. Bernstein ’83
spoke in January with Deborah Prentice, chairwoman of the psychology department,
about stereotypes — particularly gender stereotypes, one of her
academic specialties.
Much has been written about Hillary Clinton’s likability.
How does that play into gender stereotypes?
Self-promoting women — and all political candidates have to be
extremely self-promoting — run into problems because they are seen
as not likable enough. Of course, if they are not self-promoting, they
are more likable but are not up to the job. Whichever way Hillary goes,
she’s going to run into difficulties, given that we don’t
settle for competence in our political candidates; we want them to be
likable as well. It is difficult, given existing gender norms, for women
to be self-promoting and likable at the same time. George [W.] Bush played
on his likability, but the female version of that is something that evokes
the stereotype of weakness. I can understand why women do better in parliamentary
systems, where they don’t have to go directly to the people all
the time.
You have said that women feel much more negatively toward dominant
women than men do. Can you elaborate?
Women don’t like dominant behavior, whether it’s a man or
a woman who is showing it. They think everybody should be agreeable and
cooperative, and of course dominance drives these qualities out. So it
is true that they don’t like dominant women, but they don’t
like dominant men either. Men have more positive reactions to dominance
in general, especially when it’s a man who is being dominant. They
tend to have mixed reactions toward dominant women, and more positive
reactions toward dominant men.
Much was made of the moment when Hillary Clinton choked up before
the New Hampshire primary. What did you think of that?
It illustrates how stereotypes work. It’s not that people come
in and apply stereotypes wholesale to situations, but when events happen,
stereotypes provide a lens through which to interpret them. I’ve
seen George Bush’s eyes well up many times. In him, it’s seen
as his humanity showing through. But when Hillary did it at the diner,
and especially in response to another woman asking her how she does it,
it was not a man’s humanity showing through but a woman’s
vulnerability. And it seems to have worked both for her and against her.
Yet in 1972, Sen. Edmund Muskie cried before the New Hampshire primary,
and many people say it destroyed his candidacy. Has our perception of
a candidate’s emotions changed?
It really depends on the context. Certainly people want their leaders
to be human in positive ways, but they don’t want weakness. Yes,
times have changed, but I do not think people have become more tolerant
of weakness in their political leaders.
Have we had the same problem with racial stereotypes in this campaign?
My perception is that, when it comes to their public images, Hillary
Clinton is much more her gender than Barack Obama is his race. That is,
in public discussions and the media, racial stereotypes seem to come up
less often as a way to explain his behavior than gender stereotypes do
to explain hers. It is an interesting difference.
How does Bill Clinton’s role in the campaign affect perceptions?
If Hillary doesn’t trot Bill out, he’ll be the power behind
her, whereas if she does trot him out, he’ll soak up all the attention.
She’s in a difficult position.
Princeton has been coeducational for almost 40 years. Are the expectations
the same for male and female students?
No, male and female students are not perceived and evaluated in the
same way. I did a study several years ago in which I asked students about
gender stereotypes at Princeton. Princeton men are still held to traditional
male stereotypes: They’re still supposed to be ambitious and driven,
and not to be gullible or weak. That has changed surprisingly little.
For women, it’s more of a mix. They are expected to be warm and
friendly, consistent with traditional female stereotypes. They are allowed
to be competent and goal-directed, but these qualities are not stressed
for them as much as they are for men. In many ways, things are more open
for women. After graduation, they can go to Wall Street or medical school
if they want to, but they can also teach or travel. The socially approved
options are much narrower for men. I think Princeton women experience
this openness in two ways. On the one hand it’s freeing, but on
the other hand, they wonder why expectations aren’t as high for
them. It’s not sexism, exactly, but a kind of soft bigotry.